(Published in the South China Morning Post on 7 January 2007)
Poor decision-making is largely to blame
for the dreadful returns realised by US mutual fund investors. That is the
conclusion of the Dalbar study, a yearly analysis of fund flows and fund
performance first published in 1994.
From 1984 to 2002, US equity mutual
fund investors earned an average annual return of 2.6 per cent compared with
12.2 per cent for the S&P 500 Index.
Dalbar estimated that 2.9 percentage
points of the difference were due to fund underperformance and operating costs
while a staggering 6.7 percentage points were due to poor decision-making by
investors, specifically poor timing and fund selection.
In fact, John Bogle, who founded Vanguard
Group and the first index fund, testified to the US Congress in 2003 that the
returns were even worse on a dollar-weighted basis. Accounting for the huge
sums that investors poured into "hot" technology funds in 1999 and
2000 at the top of the market, Mr Bogle estimated that returns were
significantly in the red over the period.
Behavioural finance experts explain
this poor decision-making in terms of our natural tendency towards
overconfidence and bias. Researchers say people consistently overrate their
knowledge and skill.
In their paper in the Sloan Management
Review in 1992, Edward Russo and Paul Schoemaker presented the results of their
tests in which securities analysts and fund managers were posed a series of questions
and, in addition to being asked for a precise answer, were also asked for a
range in which they were 90 per cent sure the actual answer resided. On
average, the analysts chose ranges wide enough to accommodate the correct
answer only 64 per cent of the time. Fund managers were even less successful at
50 per cent. Groups that very accurately calibrated their confidence levels
included weather forecasters, bookmakers and professional bridge players.
So how does this overconfidence among
fund investors manifest itself when it comes to. decisions to buy and sell
mutual funds? Overconfidence impedes performance because investors consider outcome
ranges that are too narrow. For example, if markets have fallen, investors
significantly underestimate the likelihood of them bouncing. And the belief
that a "hot" fund will continue to be so often becomes dogmatic.
Paradoxically, overconfidence prevents
you from being aware of your overconfidence. I hear myself saying, "No!
You are wrong! I am certainly not overconfident," in response to being so
accused. We need to be shown the evidence.
Here goes. In a room of 30 people,
what is the probability that two people share the same birthday? One in 100?
One in 200? It can't be more than 10 per cent, can it? Well, it's actually 71
per cent. Surprised?
An example of erroneous perception
is provided by "the Monty Hall problem", named for the host of US
TV's Let's Make A Deal: you are on a game show where the objective is to win a
car. The host shows you three doors and says there is a car behind one of them and
a goat behind each of the other two. He asks you to pick a door. You pick a
door but it is not opened. Then the host, who knows what is behind each door,
opens one of the two you didn't pick to reveal a goat. He then offers you the
chance to change your pick to the other unopened door. What should you do?
The problem was sent to Parade magazine's
Ask Marilyn column in 1990. Author Marilyn vos Savant answered that you should
always switch doors as this doubled your chances of winning from one in three
to two in three. There was an avalanche of letters to the magazine, some from
writers with a PhD in mathematics, saying she was wrong, and accusing her of
lowering education standards. (Incidentally, Mrs vos Savant held the Guinness
world record for the world's highest IQ from 1986 until 1989, when the ranking
was abolished.)
You may think, as I did at first, that
Mrs vos Savant is wrong and that switching doors wouldn't alter the odds.
Surely, if there are two doors left, the chances are 50-50 either way, right?
Wrong. I spent an entire evening trying to persuade one of my smarter friends
that by switching doors you increase your odds. Not only did he disagree, but he
was convinced he was right. I resorted to tearing up three pieces of paper,
marking one with a cross, and repeatedly playing the game until he saw
empirically the switching strategy did in fact double the chances of winning.
Once you are able to recognize the
overconfidence that causes errors in your perception, you will be in a better
position to make more sensible investment decisions.
The problem is that overconfidence
otherwise helps us in our daily battle for survival. But while the need to
appear competent and confident might help, for example, in securing a job
promotion, it hinders sound decision-making in stock market investing.
Whatever your assessment of your own
confidence, you would do well to remember that few people think they are
below-average drivers. Fewer still think they are below-average lovers. To the
real Schumachers and Casanovas out there, I salute you, as I suspect you can
get by without really needing to be any good at investing.
For the rest of you, I suggest you keep asking yourself why your answer could be wrong and why other answers could be right.
For the rest of you, I suggest you keep asking yourself why your answer could be wrong and why other answers could be right.
No comments:
Post a Comment